Description

The open-field tower maze (OFTM) is a unique behavioral task in neuroscience used to examine the nature and mechanism of spatial learning and memory in rodents; with specific emphasis on place- and response-learning. The inserts are to be used with any Morris Water maze. Price does not include the Morris Water maze, which can be purchased separately

Price & Dimensions

Full Length Channel

Tower Set (mouse)

$ 590

For Modification
  • Removable
  • Made to fit MWM

Tower Set (Rat)

$ 690

For Modification
  • Removable
  • Made to fit MWM

Are you an academic scientist and creator?

Learn how your lab can tech transfer this and similar devices from your lab to industry.

Documentation

Introduction

The open-field tower maze (OFTM) is a unique behavioral task in neuroscience used to examine the nature and mechanism of spatial learning and memory in rodents; with specific emphasis on place- and response-learning.  The task is conducted in a way that does not stress the subjects; as stress can alter the result of the experiment (Luine et al., 1994; Beck & Luine, 2010). The OFTM paradigm is based on the fact that a non-stressed rat is able to navigate through a maze with the aid of spatial cues (i.e. place-learning); and make a specific turn response (either to the right or left to find a food reward), irrespective of its position relative to spatial cues (i.e. response-learning).

Central to this behavioral task is the fact that rodents easily remember spatial locations, especially when spatial cues like food are used as incentive. The OFTM consists of a circular platform with food towers fixed at certain points around the central area of its perimeter, one of which contains the food reward. For place-learning, the task is similar to other paradigm such as Morris Water Maze, Y-maze and T-maze that require the use of hippocampal-dependent spatial reference memory. However, response-learning is dependent on the striatum (Poldrack and Packard, 2003). Basically, this ability to remember the location of the target tower can be effected by the administration of certain drugs (e.g estradiol) or in disease models (Lipatova et al., 2014).

Cole et al., (2007), traced the theories of learning back to Hull (1943) with particular emphasis on the learning of motor responses; and how mazes such has the cross maze has been used to better parse either place or response learning in rats since 1946 by Tolman, Ritchie and Kalish. The OFTM is an improved version of other open-field mazes developed hitherto to assess spatial learning in rodents.

Apparatus and Equipment

The OFTM apparatus consists of four food towers set 54 cm apart, glued to the center portion of a circular floor 183 cm in diameter. The circular floor has a boundary wall constructed from white laminate, and 40 cm high with inside painted with different patterns, such that one quarter of the wall is painted with black and white stripes, one, solid black; one quarter, white with black circles, and the last quarter solid white. The floor of the maze is constructed with black laminate and glued to the top of plastic table. The four square-shaped food towers with dimension 8 cm x 8 cm x 20 cm; are made from white PVC post. The tops of these towers are covered with black laminate; and a black opaque plastic food cup, 2.5 cm in diameter and 2 cm deep, created from a 35 mm film canister is  attached to the center of the top of each tower with a small screw. Each food cup has a screw-top lid with three small venting holes (3 mm diameter). This ensures that the rat cannot locate the target food tower using olfactory cues. Also, the height of the towers makes it impossible for a rat to investigate the content of a food cup without rearing.

For pre-training purposes, three temporary “pre-training towers” made of the same material like the food tower but different in heights (5 cm, 10 cm and 15 cm high respectively) are taped to the maze floor during the pre-training phase.

The testing area should be sufficiently but dimly lit to allow animals to see and explore their surroundings while avoiding stress from bright lights. A sound machine is used to play white noise (approx. 70dB) in the background.

A mounted video camera like Noldus Ethovision XT is used to record the experiments from above the maze. Tracking software can be used to follow the moments of the animals within the maze. Live scoring can also be performed.

Training Protocol

The purpose of the OFTM is to assess both behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of spatial learning in animals and applicable in the study of place- and response-learning in rats. Typically, in the case of place-learning, animals can remember the specific food tower they retrieved food reward from previously; while for response-learning, they can make the right response (right or left turn) they made to previously to retrieve food reward from a tower.

Pre-Training for the Open-field Tower Maze

To motivate food-scavenging behaviors, experimental animals are food deprived the night prior to testing. Meanwhile, animals should maintain about 90% of their free-feeding body weight throughout food rationing and the testing period.

To begin the training and testing process, prepare the apparatus by ensuring it is clean and odor-free. Place a food reward (fruit pebble) in each uncapped food cup on the seven food towers. Then, place the animal in any random location of the maze and allow it to find and consume all seven food rewards.

Place-training & Response-training Procedures

For training purpose, the four food towers are baited with food reward but three are covered and one (the target tower) left open per time. Also, the rats are separated into place-learning and response-learning groups.

Mark four start locations (SL1-4) in the OFTM and assign each rat two of these start locations. Any of these locations serve as the release point. The start location/ release point within and between each training day, the type of learning (place vs response), the “target tower” (for place learners) and the turn each response learner is trained to make should be counterbalanced during the training and testing phases.

For place training, the same target food tower is used throughout. The rat could be released from any of the two start locations. For example, with food reward placed on T4, the rat is released from SL2 or SL4; or with T1 as target tower, SL1 or SL3 is used as release point.  Place-learners solve the OFTM by finding the specific location using the spatial cues. This is shown in Figure 1A below.

For response training, switch the target food tower based on the start location. For example, to get right turn, use T4 as target tower and SL3 as release point or T2 use SL2 as release point for the rat. However, to get left turn response, with a different rat, T2 is used as the target tower and SL1 used as release point or T1 as target tower and SL4 as the release position. A response-learners solves the OFTM by learning to make a specific turn (e.g., always turn right), notwithstanding the start location in the maze. This is shown in Figure 1B below.

Figure 1: Schematic for training rats on place- and response-learning.

Note: T1-4 = Food towers 1-4; SL1-4 = Start locations 1-4

All rats are subjected to 4 trials per day for 12 days (i.e. a total of 48 training trials) until they are able to achieve about 80% correct first choice response. Thereafter, testing phase is begun.

Testing and Evaluation of Place- and Response-learning using the OFTM

To test for persistency of learned behavior during the trial phase, a new start location is used for the rat. This is the start location switch test. For this version of the task, place-learners with initial start location SL2 and SL3 and had T4 as target tower, start location is changed to SL1 and SL4 with T4 remaining the target food tower. The animals are able to locate the target food tower notwithstanding this change of their start location. This is shown in figure 2A.

For a response-learner  that was initially released from start location SL2 or SL3 and learned to turn right to get food reward on T2 and T4 respectively, the start location in this case is changed  to either SL1 and SL4 with either T1 or T3 as the target food towers respectively so as to ensure right turn also. This is shown in figure 2B below.

Place-Learners Start Location Switch Test    

Figure 2: Schematic for start location switch test place- and response-learning.

For evaluation of either place- or response-learning, the number of responses (indicated by the rat rearing up over the food cup) a rat makes to find the target food tower during each trial is counted. The percentage of first choice responses within each training day is calculated to construct learning curves across days. Also, the number of trials it took a rat to achieve an a priori set criterion is calculated. An a priori criterion of first choice responding of 8 out of 10 consecutive trials may be used.

Modifications

The OFTM task has been modified to assess long-term memory retention of place- and response-learning (Liang et al., 1998; and Lipatova et al., 2006). In this case, rats are given 21 days interval after the 48 training trials. The interval period is used to assess subjects’ retention of long-term memory. Within the interval period they are left in their cage without handling.

Alternatively, the OFTM model is applicable in assessing reversal learning (ie. ability to learn new maze skills) in rats.

Sample Data

A sample data can be visualized by comparing the effects of chronic and cyclic estradiol treatments on place- and response-learning in ovariectomized female rats. To assess effect of chronic estradiol treatment, 10 μg/kg is subcutaneously administered to rat daily (continuous conditioning) as against a sham control where a placebo (peanut oil) is administered. However, for the cyclic treatment, estradiol injections are given at intervals (like every 4th day) and the placebo on the other days. In this experiment, half the rats in each condition were trained with place-training procedures and half were trained with response-training procedures. Basically, estradiol induces differential effects on spatial learning and memory (Lipatova et al. 2014). Rats under cyclic estradiol treatment require fewer treatments to reach test criterion than those in sham control or continuous conditioning. However, while the effect on response-learning is negligible for both cyclic and chronic treatment, it takes fewer trials to for subjects to get test criterion in these two treatments than in the sham control. .

Strengths & Limitations

The strength and uniqueness of the OFTM is threefold. Firstly, unlike other paradigms, it ensures that the subjects are non-stressed. In fact, this makes the model an exceptional choice in assessing spatial learning under stress-free conditions. The absence of stressors and familiarization with the maze before testing allow for better observations of working memory in the animals as they perform in the maze. Secondly, the OFTM measures learning using the percentage of first choice responses the rat makes to find the food reward. This is significant as other dependent measures common to other mazes (e.g. Morris Water Maze and Barnes Maze) like speed or distance a rat covered to reach its target location are easily affected by factors not related to learning. Lastly, the possibility of choosing the correct (target) tower naturally is reduced to 25% because of the way they are constructed; hence it reduces the possibilities of the animal to use any strategy other than spatial learning to solve the maze. Thus, there is increased level of objectivity using the OFTM to assess spatial learning than in other mazes.

A limitation of the OFTM is that although it is designed to assess spatial learning in rodents, it has only been used to assess place- and response-learning in adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Thus, apart from extrapolations, nothing can be said about performance of mice or other strains of rat.

Like other mazes that measure aspects of learning and memory, the OFTM takes cognizance of the fact that subjects’ behavior in the maze is affected by many different processes. However, the OFTM has been specifically developed to test spatial learning in rodents without the experimenter having to consider the role of stress as a confounding variable (Lipatova et al., 2015).

Summary and Key Points

  • The open-field tower maze is a unique task used to test spatial learning like place- and response-learning in rats. Unlike other alternative mazes, it removes the confounding “stress” factor.
  • This task requires trained animals to navigate maze and locate a specific tower (for place-learning) or make a specific correct turn (either right or left) for response learning.
  • Spatial cues within the maze are used to aid the animals in remembering the location of the target tower.
  • Drugs and hormones like estradiol show differential effects on spatial learning but the effect on either place or response-learning in animals differ depending on whether it is chronic or cyclic treatment.

References

Beck KD, Luine VN. 2010. Evidence for sex-specific shifting of neural processes underlying learning and memory following stress. Physiol Behav. 99 2: 204–211.

Cole, M.R., Clipperton, A., and Walt, C., Place versus response learning in rats. Learn Behav., 2007. 35 (4): p. 214-224. doi:10.3758/BF03206427 (2007).

Liang, K.C., et al., Buspirone impaired acquisition and retention in avoidance tasks: involvement of the hippocampus. Chin J Physiol., 41 (1):p. 33-44. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.07.009 (1998).

Lipatova, O., Campolattaro, M.M., Toufexis, D.J., Mabry, E.A. Place and Response Learning in the Open-field Tower Maze. J. Vis. Exp.(104), e53227, doi:10.3791/53227 (2015).

Lipatova, O., et al., Effects of continuous vs. cycling estrogen replacement on the acquisition, retention and expression of place- and response-learning in the open-field tower maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 114: p. 81-89. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.05.001 (2014).

Lipatova, O., et al., Recency-to-primacy shift in cue competition. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 32 (4): p. 396-406. doi: 2006-12924-006 [pii] 10.1037/0097-7403.32.4.396 (2006).

Luine, V., et al., Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res. 639 (1), p. 167-170. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91778-7 (1994).

Poldrack, R.A. and Packard, M.G., Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia. 2003. 41 (3): p. 245-251. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00157-4 (2003).